Autistic Empathy Toward Autistic Others

109 6
Autistic Empathy Toward Autistic Others

Discussion

Rating Data for Self- and Other Judgments


According to the behavioral results, the ASD group provided Yes responses for autistic characters more than the TD group did, whereas the TD group provided Yes responses for non-autistic characters more than the ASD group did. Thus, ASD and TD groups responded with affirmative answers to characters similar to themselves. However, the ASD group did not answer Yes for autistic characters more frequently than they did for non-autistic characters, whereas the TD group answered Yes for non-autistic characters more frequently than they did for autistic characters.

Subjective measurement may not be suitable for individuals with ASD. SRS can provide ratings from parents, teachers, spouses, other relatives or friends; it is difficult for individuals with ASD to monitor themselves using self-report scales. These results reflect a relative lack of self-awareness in ASD (Toichi et al., 2002). For example, children with ASD exhibit less self-consciousness; furthermore, autobiographical memories, which are experienced by the self, are remembered less well compared with events happening to others (Millward et al., 2000; Bruck et al., 2007; Lind, 2010; Williams and Happé, 2010). Additionally, the self-reference effect in memory is reduced in adults with ASD (Toichi et al., 2002; Lombardo et al., 2007). Taken together, these findings indicate that the strong differentiation observed in the TD group and the total lack of differentiation observed in the ASD group are both well supported.

We found no significant differences between the ASD and TD groups with regard to reaction times, and reaction times for other judgments were longer than those for self-judgments in both groups. Thus, for both groups, the cognitive load was greater for other judgments than for self-judgments. Our similarity hypothesis was not supported by the behavioral data in that selective responses toward similar targets were not observed.

Interaction Between Group and Character


To address the question of whether individuals with ASD show specific responses for others with ASD, the interaction between group (with or without ASD) and character (autistic or non-autistic) must be examined. The two-way interaction (group × character) was evaluated. According to the activation data for the interaction between group and character, the vmPFC was activated in the ASD and TD groups during self- and other judgments when the ASD group judged characters with autistic traits and the TD group judged characters without autistic traits. The findings of this study suggest that both individuals with ASD and TD individuals make selective neural responses toward others who are similar to themselves. Although individuals with ASD showed a relative lack of self-consciousness in their explicit subjective ratings, the selective activation in response to similar others with ASD reflected in the brain imaging data may suggest an implicit identification with similar others.

According to previous studies with ASD and TD participants, the vmPFC distinguished between self- and other evaluations in TD adults but not in individuals with ASD (Lombardo et al., 2010a; Pfeifer et al., 2013). However, these previous studies used a fictional character (Harry Potter) as the other target, and this character was not similar to the participants. In this study, the vmPFC activations in both ASD and TD groups were significantly greater when judging matched (autistic characters for individuals with ASD and non-autistic characters for TD individuals) than mismatched targets (autistic characters for TD individuals and non-autistic characters for individuals with ASD). Thus, individuals with ASD did not have vmPFC dysfunction in terms of the ability to distinguish between the self and another person, and the vmPFC seemed to underpin the ability to make distinctions between ASD and TD targets.

Another previous study also found that vmPFC activation did not distinguish between self- and other judgments in adults with ASD (Kennedy and Courchesne, 2008), although the 'other' used in this design was someone with whom participants were likely to be very close: their mother. The vmPFC activation is related to processing similar others (Schmitz et al., 2004; Ochsner et al., 2005; Jenkins et al., 2008; Chen et al., 2010; Krienen et al., 2010), which is consistent with our finding that the vmPFC was activated in response to the self and similar others in both the TD group and the ASD group.

It is important to note that the IFG, postcentral gyrus, paracentral lobule, precuneus, cuneus, lingual gyrus, cerebellum, fusiform and SFG, as well as vmPFC were activated during this study. All these areas were larger clusters than the vmPFC. Although this study focused on the vmPFC funtion, future studies need to further investigate the functions of these areas.

Differences in Connectivity


According to the functional connectivity results, frontal–posterior connectivity (Just et al., 2012) was observed in TD individuals, but not in individuals with ASD, during similar judgments (autistic judgments in the ASD group and non-autistic judgments in the TD group). The present findings reflect differences in the type of brain connectivity exhibited by the ASD and TD groups. Although the activation results for both groups revealed that the vmPFC was activated in response to similar others, the functional connectivity results reflected a specific network in each group, i.e. responses in individuals with ASD toward autistic characters and responses in TD individuals toward non-autistic characters. Although empathic responses in TD individuals are based on collaboration between frontal (the vmPFC as the area for self-representation) and posterior (the STG; Wernicke's area for language processing) areas, empathic responses in individuals with ASD are based on collaboration within frontal areas.

Several previous studies have demonstrated a lack of empathy and deficits in self-related representation, in ASD (Greimel et al., 2010; Lombardo et al., 2010; Schulte-Rüther et al., 2011). However, previous studies also showed that it was difficult for those with ASD to assume the perspectives of TD others. To our knowledge, few studies have employed characters with and without autistic traits as target stimuli (Komeda et al., 2013a). It is difficult for TD individuals to understand others who are dissimilar from themselves (Komeda et al., 2013b). Thus, it is not surprising that it is also difficult for those with ASD to understand TD individuals. This is the first empirical study to investigate the empathy of ASD individuals for others with autistic traits (for a review, Dern, 2008). Individuals with ASD are likely to empathize with other people with ASD. Empathy varies as a function of similarity between participants and characters (Komeda et al., 2013b). Individuals with ASD and TD individuals with high levels of autistic traits, even if they have not been diagnosed with ASD, are likely to better understand others with autism. Interestingly, even when characters had autistic traits, individuals with ASD seemingly did not use the frontal–posterior network during self- and other judgments.

Source...
Subscribe to our newsletter
Sign up here to get the latest news, updates and special offers delivered directly to your inbox.
You can unsubscribe at any time

Leave A Reply

Your email address will not be published.